|
||||
The unformatted htm text below is from |
Copyright © 2004 by Leonard Evans |
The text below is © Leonard Evans. Portions totaling not more than 400 words my be freely used with appropriate citation. |
For more extensive use, permission in writing must be first obtained by emailing the author |
3 Overview of traffic fatalities
This html version contains only the text (no figures, tables equations, or summary and conclusions). To check printed book appearance see pdf version of Chapter 1 or pdf version of Chapter 16.
The beginnings
The first automobile propelled by an
internal combustion engine is generally considered to be a
three-wheeled vehicle introduced in 1886 by Germany's Karl
Benz. Vehicle development thereafter proceeded rapidly in
Europe and in the US. A revolutionary development occurred
in 1913 near Detroit, Michigan, when Henry Ford introduced
the moving assembly line to mass produce the Ford Model T.
The assembly line so reduced production costs that vehicles
could be offered at prices that a substantial portion of the
US public could afford, leading to rapid growth in vehicle
ownership. As the number of vehicles increased, so did the
number of crashes and the number of fatalities.
The first known fatal traffic crash
killed a 44-year-old female pedestrian on 17 August 1896 in
London. The first fatal crash to be well documented and
photographed occurred on 25 February in 1899 in Harrow, near
London. A detailed account of it appears in the 4 March 1899
issue of the The Autocar, a British weekly started in 1895
and still being published. Six men were traveling in the
vehicle shown in Fig. 3-1 when the driver applied maximum
braking as the vehicle gained speed on a down hill. This
caused the tires to separate from the wheels, with
subsequent wheel failure. The vehicle dropped to the ground,
coming to an abrupt stop. In accord with well-known physical
laws, all six occupants continued to move forward until
stopped when their bodies struck the ground or other objects
in the roadway environment.
The driver died instantly from head
injuries, and one passenger died after spending more than
three days unconscious in the hospital. The inquest into the
two deaths focused on factors remarkably similar to those
that would arise today. Many questions were asked about
driver alcohol consumption prior to the crash (no more than
modest quantities were consumed, and well prior to the
crash). Witnesses attested to the skill of the driver, one
stating "he is a splendid driver." However, based
on testimony by many witnesses, the coroner advised the
jury, "the car appeared to be going at too rapid a pace
to be safe, either for the occupants themselves or the
public." The quality of the tires and the spokes of the
wheel were criticized as contributory factors to the deaths.
In the investigation of this early
crash, important themes at the core of traffic safety were
already being recognized, including:
· Tension between technology and how it is used
· Tension between driver skill and how it is used
Figure 3-1. The first well documented fatal crash
Long term trends
Figure 3-2 illustrates the
rapid growth in fatalities as motorization increased in the
US. The general pattern applies to other countries that
began to motorize later than the US, and seems likely to
occur in the future in countries now in the early stages of
motorization. As US vehicle ownership increased rapidly, so
did traffic deaths, peaking at 54,589 in 1972, and declining
later to a fairly stable rate of just over 40,000 per year.
Annual traffic fatalities are
increasing rapidly in China in the same way as happened in
the US in the 1920s. There is no way to reliably estimate
what the maximum number of fatalities per year will be in
China, or when that maximum will occur. However, current
trends indicate ongoing increases. Other countries in the
midst of rapid motorization are experiencing corresponding
increases in traffic deaths.
Deaths for the same distance of travel (distance rate)
The number of traffic
deaths per year shows little in the way of an interpretable
pattern. However, if we instead examine the number of
traffic deaths in the US for the same distance of vehicle
travel (the distance rate), the clear trend in Fig. 3-3
emerges. A straight line on the log-linear scale corresponds
to a constant percent change each year. Ever since 1921,
when data on the total distance traveled by all vehicles
were first collected, the distance rate has trended
downwards at an average decrease of about 3.5% per year.
Figure 3-4 shows the percent change
in the distance rate in a given year compared to the
previous year (that is, the percent difference between each
of the 82 pairs of consecutive points plotted in Fig. 3-3).
Of the 81 resulting differences, 64 are negative, indicating
a reduction in the rate from that of the previous year, with
an average reduction over all 81 values of 3.5% per year.
There is no obvious trend in the data in Fig. 3-4, so that
if such a trend-free pattern persists, the number of
fatalities for the same distance of travel will continue to
decline at about 3.5% per year.
The 2002 rate of 9.4 US traffic
deaths per billion km of travel is 94% below the 1921 rate
of 150. If the 1921 rate had applied in 2002, the number of
US traffic fatalities in 2002 would have exceeded half a
million. The downward trend in the distance rate is also
observed in other countries. , (p 155)
The variable that is the basis of Figs 3-3 and 3-4 is
available only after a nation establishes a procedure to
estimate the distance traveled by all of its vehicles. Even
when available, estimates of distance of travel differ
widely in reliability from country to country. The distance
traveled by all US vehicles is estimated by summing
estimates obtained in different ways from each of the
states. While this process may provide a fairly accurate
measure of year-to-year percent changes, the value
(estimated at 4.60 billion km for 2002) is of uncertain
absolute accuracy. The best estimates are for Great Britain,
based on observations at 50 sites supported by the
Department of Transport.
Deaths per registered vehicle (the
vehicle rate)
The registration, and thereby counting, of vehicles is
routinely performed by nearly all jurisdictions for taxation
purposes, and is considered fairly reliable even from the
earliest days of motorization. Figure 3-5 compares the
vehicle and distance rates for the US. Fatalities per
vehicle have been declining at an average rate of 3.3% per
year since the early 1920s, compared to 3.5% for fatalities
for the same distance of travel.
If the average distance traveled
per vehicle remained constant from one year to the next,
then the two plots in Fig. 3-5 would be identical (apart
from a scaling factor). They differ to the extent that they
do because the average distance traveled per year per
vehicle has been increasing since the early days of
motorization (Fig. 4-13, p. 91).
Figure 3-6 shows that not only does
the vehicle rate vary by large amounts from country to
country, but it also changes at different rates. The
fatality rate has been declining in China at 10 percent per
year, or halving in 7 years. The US decline of 3.3% per year
corresponds to halving in 21 years. One might conclude that
the explanation is simply that it is easier to achieve a
high rate decline when the absolute value is higher.
However, this is not the complete explanation, as the data
for Sweden show. Even after achieving a lower absolute value
than that for the US, the Swedish rate continued to decline
faster than the US rate. We compare changes in rates in
different countries in detail
in Chapter 15.
Figure 3-6. Traffic fatalities per thousand registered vehicles for the US, China, and Sweden.
Fatality rates versus degree of motorization. The US
vehicle-rate values in Fig. 3-6 are shown again in Fig. 3-7,
but plotted versus the number of vehicles per thousand
population. The number of vehicles per capita can be
regarded as a measure of degree of motorization, and may
therefore offer insight into why fatality rates decline. In
the US and GB, vehicle registrations per capita in any year
have nearly always exceeded the rate in the previous year.
Exceptions are related to periods of economic depression or
war.
Extended time series as in Fig. 3-7
are not available for many countries. However, nearly all
countries have available counts of fatalities, registered
vehicles, and human population for some recent years. The
number of deaths per thousand vehicles varies greatly
between countries - from under 0.13 for Japan, Norway,
Sweden, Great Britain, and the Netherlands to 65 for
Mozambique, a factor of over 400 (Table 3-1 and Fig. 3-8).
The data are
mainly from Refs and , but augmented by personal
communications from colleagues in a number of countries.
There is a general tendency for the
number of traffic fatalities per vehicle to be lower the
higher the degree of motorization, although departures of
50% from the trend occur. The relationship between different
countries observed at similar times (Fig. 3-8) has features
in common with the relationship for the data for one country
observed at different times (Fig. 3-7).
Note that the US, with over three
motorized vehicles for every four people, is the most
motorized country in the world but has far from the lowest
fatality rate, a theme to which we return in Chapter 15.
Who is killed?
All the discussion above has
focused on the total numbers killed per year, without regard
to category of road user. Although essentially every traffic
crash involves at least one driver, Table 3-2 (based on FARS
2001)4 shows that 38% of those killed are not drivers. (Rare
crashes with no drivers occur, such as when a child alone in
a vehicle sitting in a passenger seat, sets it in motion).
The patterns in Table 3-2 change only moderately from year
to year (compare with data for 19883(p 45)). However, this
pattern should be interpreted to apply specifically to the
US. The percent of fatalities that are drivers is much lower
in earlier stages of motorization.
Male fatalities in the US in 2001
totaled 28,878, compared to 13,168 female fatalities, giving
a male-to-female ratio of 2.19 to 1 The World Health
Organization estimates that of 1,194,115 people killed in
2001 in traffic worldwide, 848,234 were male compared to
345,881 female, giving a male-to-female ratio of 2.34 to 1.
The predominance of male fatalities in all types of injury
deaths is a universal phenomenon, applying to essentially
all types of non-disease deaths, including firearm and other
homicides, suicide, drowning, and falling. The preponderance
of male over female traffic fatalities persists at all ages
(Fig. 3-9). This is not exclusively a driver phenomenon.
Figure 3-10 shows that 60% of non-driver (passenger,
pedestrian, etc.) road user fatalities in the US were male.
(Relationships focusing on ages of drivers are given in
Chapter 7).
Trends in pedestrian fatalities
The percent of all traffic fatalities that are pedestrians
declines as nations motorize (Fig. 3-11). Note how similar
the curves are between the US and Canada on the one hand,
and GB and Ireland on the other, reflecting readily
observable differences in urban landscape and walking
patterns in everyday life. The percent of all fatalities
that are pedestrians is much higher in less motorized
countries (for example, 80% in Kenya).
Missing values in data sets
Of the 42,116 fatalities coded in FARS 2001, 25,840, or
61.35%, were coded as drivers. The reason why this percent
is not identical to the 61.59% in Table 3-2 illustrates
features common to all large traffic data sets - nearly all
variables have missing (or unknown) values. There were 105
fatalities classified as vehicle occupants, but no
information was available identifying whether they were
drivers or passengers. Such lack of identification can arise
in the case of vehicle fires, or if multiple occupants are
ejected from vehicles, and so on. In Table 3-2 these
unidentified occupants were distributed among drivers and
passengers in the same proportion as identified drivers and
passengers so that the distribution would reflect all those
killed. This provides a better estimate of the number of
driver fatalities than the alternative of assuming that none
of the occupants of unknown type was a driver. Likewise, the
total numbers of fatalities used to produce Fig. 3-9 is
slightly less than the total in Table 3-2 because there are
cases for which gender or age is not coded.
Missing values make it impossible to achieve identical
totals from one tabulation to another. For variables like
age, gender, and vehicle model year, this is no more than an
irritating untidiness that has no material effect on
results. However, it becomes a major hurdle for analyses
using variables, such as alcohol level or belt use, which
have a large fraction of missing values.
Drivers rates are usually best measure
In addressing how factors, especially vehicle factors,
affect safety, driver rates rather than occupant rates
should be used in most cases. Focusing on drivers avoids the
confounding influence of occupancy. If vehicle A experienced
50% more occupant fatalities than vehicle B, this does not
mean that an occupant of A is at greater risk than an
occupant of B. If the average occupancy of A was 1.9, and
that of B was 1.1, then the risk to each traveler in A is
13% lower than the risk to each traveler in B. The aim
should usually be to determine driver risk. The assumption
that passenger risk is proportional to driver risk is
generally appropriate.
Number of vehicles
More drivers (and occupants) are killed in single-vehicle
crashes than in two-vehicle crashes. Much coverage of
traffic safety in the media presumes that the major risk is
from two-vehicle crashes, with the characteristics of the
other vehicle exercising a more central role in overall risk
than is consistent with the pattern in Table 3-3.
Table 3-3. Distribution of number of drivers killed
according to the number of vehicles (any type) involved in
the crash. For single-vehicle crashes
the object struck, or event, associated with most harm is
indicated.
FARS 2001.
Although under two percent of occupants killed are killed
in crashes involving more than 4 vehicles, fatal crashes
involving large numbers of vehicles do occur. In 2001, one
crash involving 56 vehicles killed three drivers and three
passengers. There were also fatal crashes involving 15, 16,
31, and 57 vehicles, each crash killing one driver (one
passenger was also killed in the 15-vehicle crash).
The most harmful event is indicated
in Table 3-3 for single-vehicle crashes. If, say, a vehicle
strikes a curb, and subsequently overturns leading to
occupant ejection, then overturn is likely to be coded in
FARS as the most harmful event for that vehicle. This
variable is not listed in Table 3-3 for multiple-vehicle
crashes because it is associated with vehicles, not crashes.
Another variable, the first harmful event, is associated
with the crash. The first harmful event might be two
vehicles striking each other, leading to one of them
subsequently overturning. For this vehicle the most harmful
event might be the overturn, and for the other, striking
another vehicle.
The most harmful event associated
with 40% of the drivers killed in single-vehicle crashes is
overturn (or rollover). Most of the other most harmful
events involve striking objects that are part of the
extended roadway environment. The most commonly struck
object leading to death is a tree, reflecting the large
number of trees adjacent to roadways. The all other category
includes more than 30 additional most harmful events listed
in FARS. For vehicles involved in multiple-vehicle crashes,
striking another vehicle is the most harmful event for 90%
of the fatalities.
Fraction of deaths due to rollover and ejection
Figure 3-12 shows the percent of
fatalities in light trucks and cars in which overturn was
the most harmful event. Given a driver death, rollover is
much more likely to be the most harmful event for a driver
of a light truck than for the driver of a car. While this
likely reflects the typically higher center of gravity of
light trucks compared to cars, care must always be exercised
in interpreting ratios such as those in Fig. 3-12.
Reductions in risk in non-rollover crashes lead to higher
values for the percent of all deaths that are from rollover
crashes even if the risk of a rollover fatality were to
remain unchanged. The age and gender dependence shows how
much the probability that a driver death is from rollover
depends on driver as well as vehicle characteristics, being
about twice as great for 20-year-old drivers as for
70-year-old drivers, and about twice as great for light
trucks as for cars. Given a driver death, rollover is as
likely to be involved when a 20-year-old is driving a car as
when a 70-year-old is driving a light truck.
Alcohol is a large factor in
overturn crashes, with 55% of car drivers and 53% of
light-truck drivers killed in rollover crashes having blood
alcohol concentration levels exceeding 0.8%, the legal limit
in most US states. For non-rollovers the corresponding
figures are 35% and 35%. All these values are based only on
data for which the blood alcohol level was known.
Also, it should be kept in mind how
total fatality risk depends on gender and age (Fig. 3-9), so
the apparent lack of a major gender dependence in Fig. 3-12
means only that the absolute gender dependence is not
materially different from that in Fig. 3-9. Far more male
than female drivers are killed in rollover crashes, but
given a driver death, the probability it is a rollover is
not strongly gender dependent.
For the fatalities represented in Fig. 3-12, 17% of the male
drivers of light trucks wore belts (24% of females did). For
car drivers the rates were 22% for males and 26% for
females. These rates are well below the approximately 50%
rates for fatal crashes overall, which are in turn well
below rates observed in traffic (p. 52 and Chapter 11).
Many occupants are ejected and
killed in non-rollover crashes, so ejection and rollover are
separate, although related, factors. Of those killed in
rollover crashes, 54% were fully ejected from their
vehicles; 59% of fatalities with total ejection resulted
from rollover crashes. Fig. 3-13 shows the percent of all
drivers killed who were totally ejected from their vehicles.
Figure 3-13. The percent of fatally injured drivers of light trucks and cars who were fully ejected from their vehicles. Belt use by the drivers of light trucks was 1.6% for males and 4.2% for females (for cars, 6.2% and 8.2%). FARS 2001.
Belt use by ejected drivers of light trucks was 1.6% for
males and 4.2% for females. For drivers of cars, it was 6.2%
for males and 8.2% for females. Overall, 96% of the drivers
included in Fig. 3-13 were not wearing belts. Properly worn
lap/shoulder belts make ejection exceedingly improbable.
Being ejected increases fatality risk three to four times
compared to remaining in the vehicle in a same severity
crash . The effectiveness of safety belts in reducing
fatality risk when rollover is the first crash event is 82%
(p. 281-282).
Fatalities according to seating position
Figure 3-14 shows the percent
of occupants killed according to the seat they occupied. The
data are for car occupants only as the six seats represented
do not apply to vehicles in general. Indeed, many of the
cars included have only four seats, and those with six seats
tend to be older model year cars. This figure reflects the
mix of cars on the roads in 2001 by type and model year. All
the occupants in Fig. 3-14 are fatalities - so the pattern
does not represent occupancy patterns for vehicles on the
road that do not crash, for which reliable data do not seem
to be available.
The restrained category includes the use of lap and shoulder
belt, shoulder belt only, lap belt only, child safety seat
or unknown type of restraint. Those coded as not using the
devices properly are excluded. Ninety percent of restrained
drivers and right-front passengers were using the familiar
integrated lap/shoulder belt combination that became
standard for all 1974 and later model year cars. The
restraint use variable is, in general, not all that reliable
because many occupants who survive tell police officers that
they were belted when they were not. However, as all
occupants included in Fig. 3-14 are fatalities, the coded
restraint use is considered reliable. The cells show the
percent relative to the 18,216 occupants with known
restraint use killed as occupants of cars - all 12 values
sum to 100%.
Belt use by fatalities compared to observed belt rates
The data in Fig. 3-14 show
49% of driver and right-front passengers killed in cars were
belted. The corresponding value for light trucks is 31%. For
all fatalities in cars and light trucks, 43% were unbelted.
In 2001, the observed belt use for drivers and passengers
was 73%.
The difference between the wearing
rates of those killed and those observed in traffic has been
incorrectly attributed exclusively to the reduction in
fatality risk produced by belts, and the reduction
incorrectly used to estimate the effectiveness of safety
belts in reducing fatality risk in crashes. The erroneous
calculation proceeds along the following lines. If one had a
population of identical drivers experiencing random crashes,
with 73% using a protective device, then a finding of 43% of
fatalities using the device would imply that the device
reduced fatality risk by 1 - (27 43)/(73 57) = 72%. This is
not even an approximate estimate of safety-belt
effectiveness, but rather a value that is necessarily
substantially higher than the true value because three
effects bias the estimate upwards:
1. The belt wearing estimates are based on daytime
observations. It is difficult to see if people are wearing
their belts in the dark! Also, nighttime traffic is too
sparse to collect observational data efficiently, yet this
is when many fatal crashes occur (Fig. 3-19, p. 58).
Nighttime wearing rates are expected to be lower than
daytime rates.
2. Drivers who wear belts have lower crash rates than
non-wearers, so some of the reduction in deaths attributed
to the belt's effectiveness is due instead to the avoidance
of crashes.
3. When belt wearers do crash, they have lower
severity crashes than wearers.
The effects listed above are treated in detail in Chapter
11.
Relative fatality risk in different seats
The data in Fig. 3-14 do
not address the relative risk of sitting in different seats,
because the number of fatalities in a seat is determined
mainly by the occupancy of that seat. Even if we could
correct for different occupancy rates, other factors that
affect fatality risk would still make it difficult to
isolate the influence of the seating position. Cars with
only one occupant are involved in crashes of different types
and severity than those with more than one occupant.
Occupants in different seats have different distributions by
gender and age, factors that influence fatality risk in a
crash (Chapter 6). We thus encounter another example of the
problem of exposure referred to in Chapter 1.
The risk associated with different
seating positions was addressed by selecting, from 1975-1985
FARS data, cars containing drivers and passengers in
specified seats. In order to avoid confounding gender and
age effects, only cases in which the driver and passenger
were of the same gender, and had ages the same to within
three years, were included. Also, occupants coded as using
any restraint system, or who were less than 16 years old,
were excluded from the analysis. Data restricted in this way
were used to compute the ratio
3-1
R provides a largely
assumption-free estimate of the difference in risk due to
differences in the physical environment of the different
seating positions. It is essentially free from the
confounding effects that arise from occupant characteristics
being correlated with different seating positions because
all occupants for Eqn 3-1 were killed in crashes in which
the other occupant was also present in the same car involved
in the same crash, and both occupants were of the same
gender and similar age.
Raw data and computed values of R are shown in Fig. 3-15.
Because all values are relative to the driver, there is no
computed relative risk for the driver, for whom, by
definition, R = 1.
For cars containing a driver and a
right-front passenger, there were 15,880 right-front
passenger fatalities compared to 15,793 driver fatalities,
for a right-front passenger relative fatality risk of R =
1.006 ± 0.011. The error is computed assuming that the
fatalities arise from a Poisson process (p. 14). Thus, to
high precision, no difference is found in fatality risk to
drivers and right-front passengers. The center-front seat R
= 0.78 ± 0.04 indicates that this position is associated
with a (22 ± 4)% lower fatality risk than the outboard
(driver or right-front passenger) front positions. The
outboard-rear seats have a composite R = 0.739 ± 0.015.
That is, for unrestrained occupants in outboard seating
positions, rear seats are associated with a fatality risk
(26.1 ± 1.5)% lower than for front seats. The safest seat
of all is the center rear, where risk is (37 ± 3)% lower
than in the driver seat. The earlier FARS data used in this
study documented a fleet of larger cars with larger numbers
of center seats than the 2001 data used for Fig. 3-14.12
Another study using more recent vehicles reports a 39% lower
risk of fatality and 33% lower risk of injury in rear
compared to front seats.
Seating position and direction of impact
FARS data contain a
principal impact point variable, defined as the impact that
is judged to have produced the greatest personal injury or
property damage for a particular vehicle. The impact refers
to the location on the vehicle sustaining damage, so that
principal impact point at 12 o'clock means that the damage
is in the center-front of the vehicle (Fig. 3-16). The
region damaged does not necessarily indicate the direction
of impact, because the center front could be damaged by,
say, an oblique impact into a tree. A detailed post-crash
investigation is necessary to determine the actual direction
of impact. However, principal impact point 12 o'clock may be
approximately interpreted as indicating, on average at
least, head-on impacts.
Figure 3-16. Regions corresponding to impact clock points.
Figure 3-17 shows relative risk for the five passenger
seating positions versus principal impact point displayed in
the same bird's eye view of the vehicle traveling up the
page used in Figs 3-14 and 3-15. All values are relative to
a value one for drivers.
Focusing on the right-front
passenger data (top-right circle) shows that when the impact
is from the right, the right-front passenger is 2.74 times
as likely to die as is the driver (as before, both occupants
are of the same gender, and ages not different by more than
three years). When the impact is from the left, the
right-front passenger is 0.38 times as likely to die as the
driver. This can be expressed equivalently by saying that
the driver is 1/0.38 = 2.63 times as likely to die as is the
right-front passenger. The essential symmetry (reflected in
the closeness of the ratios 2.74 and 2.63) is to be expected
on physical grounds, and increases confidence in the
estimates. For principal impact point 12 o'clock the value
of R for right-front passengers is R = 0.988 ± 0.019. Thus
the similarity of fatality risk to drivers and right-front
passengers applies also to the frontal case. Drivers and
right-front passengers are at similar fatality risks from
rear impacts (R = 1.00 ± 0.10).
The safety advantage of sitting in
a rear seat compared to the corresponding front seat is
larger for frontal crashes than for all crashes without
regard to direction of impact (Fig. 3-16). The general
pattern in Fig. 3-17 shows that occupants near the point of
impact are at greater fatality risk than those far from the
point of impact. Although rear occupants are at much greater
risk than front occupants in rear impact crashes, such
crashes account for less than 5% of all fatalities. The
overall 26% lower fatality risk in rear than in front seats
reflects that frontal crashes account for most fatalities.
The lower risk in center seats is likely reflecting greater
distance from the highest risk points of impact, as well as
protective cushioning from other passengers.
A corresponding phenomenon occurs
for motorcyclists, where it is found that fatality risk in
the driver seat is (26 ± 2)% greater than that in the
passenger seat; for frontal crashes the difference is (40%
± 6)%, again demonstrating the greater risk associated with
being nearer the impact. Also, the motorcycle driver
probably helps cushion the impact for the passenger. For
non-frontal motorcycle crashes, drivers and passengers are
at similar risk (R = 1.01 ± 0.04).
An additional finding in the car occupant risk study is that
there are 38% more impacts of high severity from the right
than from the left, a result possibly reflecting asymmetries
resulting from driving on the right.12 It would be
informative to know if countries that drive on the left
experience more severe impacts from the left than from the
right.
Variation throughout year and day
Fatalities occur at a far from
uniform rate, varying systematically throughout the year, as
shown in Fig. 3-18. In each of the 8 years plotted, the
lowest number of fatalities was recorded for February, and
by an amount larger than due to the fewer number of days in
February. In all cases, the highest number of fatalities was
recorded for July or August, a pattern similarly stable in
1983-1988 data.3(p 86) The pattern occurs largely because
more difficult driving conditions in winter months reduce
speeds (see also Chapter 5).
Fatalities follow the regular daily
pattern shown in Fig. 3-19. The hour with the fewest number
of fatalities is from 4 am to 5 am on the normal workdays
Monday through Friday. The hour with the largest number of
fatalities - about four times the lowest - is from 2 am to 3
am on Saturday and Sunday mornings, with weekend drinking
playing a key role.
Someone does NOT get killed every 13 minutes
If the 42,116 US fatalities in 2001 are divided by the
number of minutes in the year the result is an average rate
of one per 12.5 minutes. This has led to many statements
like, "There is a death caused by a motor vehicle crash
every 13 minutes and a disabling injury every 14
seconds." This is far from being strictly correct. No
fatal crash occurred between 3:30 am and 7:00 am on Tuesday
6 March 2001, or between 3:00 am and 6:30 am on Tuesday 27
November 2001. In both these cases, three and a half hours
elapsed without anyone being killed. Note how these extreme
values relate to the hourly and monthly dependencies in Figs
3-18 and 3-19. On the other hand, there are many occurrences
of three, four, and five crashes being coded as occurring at
the same time. Indeed, if crashes were a perfectly random
Poisson process, more would be coded as occurring at the
same time than separated by any other time interval.
The distribution of the times
between crashes coded in FARS 2001 is plotted in Fig. 3-20.
All crash times are converted to Eastern time, so a crash
occurring at 1:00 pm California time is converted to 4:00 pm
Eastern time, and is therefore correctly interpreted as
occurring at the same time as a 4:00 pm crash in New York.
Figure 3-18. The distribution of the times between
consecutive fatal crashes in the US in 2001.
If the data were a perfect
representation of reality, the largest number of occurrences
listed above would be for time zero between crashes. As the
smallest time unit in the data is one minute, a recording of
no time difference between a pair of crashes implies that
they occurred within a minute of each other. The probability
that two crashes in fact occur at exactly the same time (or
are separated by any specified time) is, of course, zero.
Compelling statistical reasoning implies that the number of
occurrences should decline systematically with increasing
time since the previous crash. Thus fewer crashes are
expected to occur 5 minutes after the previous crash than to
occur 4 minutes after the previous crash. The prominent peak
at 5 minutes results from the tendency to record times in
multiples of 5 minutes, a tendency reinforced by traditional
mechanical analogue devices which display time using
circular dials marked in five-minute intervals. This
tendency (it may disappear when the digital revolution is
complete) also leads to the prominent cyclical pattern of
peaks at multiples of five minutes evident in Fig. 3-20. If
one assumes that about 20% of readings are rounded to the
nearest five minutes, a smoother pattern results.
The straight line in Fig. 3-20 is
the theoretical prediction based on assuming that crashes
are a Poisson process with an average risk of crashing per
minute, l = 0.07133 (calculated as 37,490 crashes divided by
the number of minutes in 2001). This corresponds to fatal
crashes occurring at an average rate of one every 14.02
minutes. The number of crashes reflects the exclusion of 372
for which the time of crash was not adequately coded (see p.
45 on missing values).
The open symbols in Fig. 3-20
represent averaging over five minutes rather than one minute
as data become sparse due to few occurrences of crash-free
periods of more than a couple of hours. The greater number
of crash-free periods of more than an hour or so observed
compared to the Poisson process prediction reflects major
departures from the assumption of a constant rate of
crashing, as is clearly apparent in the monthly and hourly
variations in Fig. 3-18 and Fig. 3-19. However, even if the
crashes were perfectly random, the relationship in Fig. 3-20
still predicts that in a year we should expect one period of
110 minutes to elapse with no crashes anywhere in the US.
Caution in interpreting averages
Much of the material in this
and other chapters relates to averages. Averages do not
apply to individuals. As shown for the case of the claim
that someone gets killed every 13 minutes, averages can
convey a misleading impression. Averages should be
interpreted with a caution well captured in the quip: An
average is like a bikini swimsuit - what it reveals is
interesting, but what it conceals is crucial. It has also
been remarked that the average human has approximately one
breast and one testicle.
Summary and conclusions (see printed text)
References for Chapter 3 - Numbers in [ ] refer to superscript references in book that do not correctly show in this html version. To see how they appear in book see the pdf version of Chapter 1.
[1] RoadPeace.
World’s first road
death.
http://www.roadpeace.org/articles/WorldFirstDeath.html[2]
Evans L. A crash course in traffic safety.
Encyclopædia Britannica Medical and Health Annual, 1997,
p.126-139.
[3] Evans L.
Traffic Safety and the Driver. New York, NY: Van
Nostrand Reinhold; 1991.
[4] Fatality Analysis
Reporting System (FARS) Web-Based Encyclopedia. Data
files and procedures to analyze them.
http://www-fars.nhtsa.dot.gov/
[5] International Road
Traffic and Accident Database (OECD) (2002), March
2004.
http://www.bast.de/htdocs/fachthemen/irtad/english/englisch.html
[6] Jacobs G, Aeron-Thomas
A. Africa road safety review final report.
Report PR/INT/659/2000 prepared by TRL (UK) for US
Department of Transportation, Federal Highway
Administration, December 2000. http://www.hsph.harvard.edu/traffic/papers/AfricaRoadSafetyReview2000.pdf
[7] World Health
Organization. Burden of Disease Project. Global
burden of disease estimates for 2001.
http://www3.who.int/whosis/menu.cfm?path=burden
[8] Odero W, Khayesi M,
Heda PM. Road traffic injuries in Kenya: Magnitude,
causes and status of intervention. Inj Control Saf
Promot. 2003; 10: 53-61.
[9] Evans L, Frick
MC. Potential fatality reductions through eliminating
occupant ejection from cars. Accid Anal Prev. 1989; 21:
169-182.
[10] The Transportation Link, October
2001. NHTSA Reports: Seat belt use by drivers and
passengers reaches 73%. http://osdbuweb.dot.gov/translink/oct2001/index8.htm
[11] Glassbrenner D. Safety belt use in 2002 – Demographic characteristics. Research note DOT HS 809.557. Washington, DC: US Department of Transportation, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration; March 2003.
http://www.nhtsa.dot.gov/people/injury/airbags/demographic03-03/demographic.htm
[12] Evans L, Frick MC. Seating
position in cars and fatality risk. Am J Pub Health.
1988: 78; 1456-1458.
[13] Smith KM, Cummings P. Passenger
seating position and the risk of passenger death or injury
in traffic crashes. Accid Anal Prev. 2004; 36; 257–260.[14]
Evans L, Frick MC. Helmet effectiveness in preventing
motorcycle driver and passenger fatalities. Accid Anal
Prev. 1988; 20: 447-458.
[15] National Safety Council.
Report on injuries in America, 6 June
2000.
http://www.nsc.org/lrs/statinfo/99report.htm